Drivers’ attitudes toward imperfect distraction mitigation strategies

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2006.02.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Studies were conducted to assess driver acceptance of and trust in distraction mitigation strategies. Previous studies have shown that in-vehicle tasks undermine driver safety, and that there is a need for strategies to reduce the effects of in-vehicle distractions. Trust and acceptance of such strategies strongly influence their effectiveness. Different strategies intended to reduce distraction were categorized in a taxonomy. Focus groups were conducted to help refine this taxonomy and explore driver acceptance issues related to these strategies. A driving simulator experiment was then conducted using two of the strategies: an advising strategy that warns drivers of potential dangers and a locking strategy that prevents the driver from continuing a distracting task. These strategies were presented to 16 middle-aged and 12 older drivers in two modes (auditory, visual) with two levels of adaptation (true, false). Older drivers accepted and trusted the strategies more than middle-aged drivers. Regardless of age, all drivers preferred strategies that provided alerts in a visual mode rather than an auditory mode. When the system falsely adapted to the road situation, trust in the strategies declined. The findings show that display modality has a strong effect on driver acceptance and trust, and that older drivers are more trusting and accepting of distraction mitigation technology even when it operates imperfectly.

Introduction

Driver distraction can be defined as the diversion of driver attention away from the driving task and can be characterized in several ways. Both a driver’s willingness to engage in a non-driving task and the attentional demands placed on the driver by that task contribute to the potential for distraction. Drivers do not always appropriately divide their attention between potentially conflicting activities, creating hazardous situations. The introduction of advanced technologies (e.g., navigational displays) to the driving domain raises additional concerns, because such systems may reduce driving safety by distracting the driver in critical situations and requiring too much driver attention (Verwey, 2000). It is, therefore, important to develop distraction mitigation strategies that will help reduce driver distraction.

Driver distraction mitigation strategies are diverse. Approaches to mitigating the effects of distraction may consider in-vehicle devices as conversational partners, and use concepts of communication theory to reduce distraction (Wiese & Lee, in press). According to this approach, distraction might be reduced if in-vehicle devices included some of the same conversational mechanisms that people use to coordinate their interactions. Another approach is to consider distraction mitigation strategies as a form of automation. Such automation adapts to the driver or to the roadway situation to encourage drivers to attend to the road and respond to critical roadway demands. Extensive research concerning automation in other domains can provide insights into how drivers may use or rely on distraction mitigation strategies that may or may not adapt appropriately (Sarter et al., 1997, Sheridan, 2002).

Trust is a particularly important factor influencing the use of and the reliance on automation, and can also impact the effectiveness of different strategies. Miscalibrated trust and the potential for misuse and disuse of automation may result in a failure to provide expected benefits (Parasuraman & Riley, 1997). As distrust may lead to the disuse of the automation, mistrust can lead to a failure to monitor the system’s behavior properly and to recognize its limitations, thereby leading to inappropriate reliance on the system (Lee & See, 2004). Over-reliance on the system might amplify risk-taking behavior as the driver places more trust in the automation. In situations of over-reliance, the failure of high levels of automation might lead to more severe safety problems than lower levels of automation. High levels of automation may also lead to lower situation awareness (Endsley, 1995) and greater dependence, thereby generating more opportunities to engage in risk-taking behavior. However, situations with time-critical elements (e.g., impending crash) do require higher levels of automation (Moray & Inagaki, 2003). If the system senses a near-fatal situation, the level of automation should be high enough to take control immediately. That is, if the driver is going to crash, the vehicle should take action.

Appropriate reliance on automation also depends on the performance of the automation and whether or not it is adapting appropriately to the driver state and situational demands. A system that falsely adapts takes action when there is no need, or takes inappropriate or no action when there is a need. False system adaptation undermines drivers’ response to and acceptance of the system, which in turn influences overall system effectiveness (Parasuraman, Hancock, & Olofinboba, 1997). False adaptation includes both false positives (an alarm given when no impending collision is present) and false negatives (an alarm not given when an impending collision is present). In these scenarios, distrust and disuse can result from high false-alarm rates. Due to the low base rate of collision events, the probability of a collision when a warning is given can be quite low, while the false-positive alarm rate can be quite high, even if the warning system is highly advanced. High false alarm rates can also lead to driver frustration, which is itself a type of emotional distraction that can undermine traffic safety (Burns & Lansdown, 2000).

False adaptation and diminished trust can undermine driver acceptance. Driver acceptance depends on ease of system use, ease of learning, perceived value, advocacy of the system or willingness to endorse, and driving performance (Stearns, Najm, & Boyle, 2002). Each of these components presents complex behavioral phenomena which impact the joint performance of the driver and in-vehicle technology. Acceptance interacts with trust such that low levels of acceptance lead to disuse. Higher levels of trust, however, do not necessarily lead to greater acceptability of technology (Siegrist, 2000). Therefore, driver acceptance of a distraction mitigation strategy should be assessed before the strategy is implemented.

Driver acceptance can also be influenced by the presentation modality. If the strategy uses an alarm or a display that is perceived as demanding (i.e., ease of use), or non-intuitive (i.e., ease of learning), acceptance will be low. Some of the most common modalities employed in warning systems and displays are visual and auditory (Wickens & Hollands, 1999). Because visual warnings demand the same cognitive resource as the driving task, these strategies may be less effective (Wickens, Lee, Liu, & Gordon, 2003). However, even though auditory warnings are omni-directional, and hence may be more effective, some sounds may be annoying (Berglund, Harder, & Preis, 1994), particularly highly urgent warnings (Wiese & Lee, 2004).

Age is also a factor that affects attitudes towards technology. In general, older adults have less positive attitudes towards technology (Brickfield, 1984, Kantowitz et al., 1997). Other studies have shown that older drivers may also put more trust in technology (Fox & Boehm-Davis, 1998). These findings suggest that older drivers’ perceptions are highly dependent on the types of technology assessed. For example, collision warning systems may directly compensate for cognitive impairments in older drivers and hence increase trust, whereas navigational displays may place greater demands on drivers and thus diminish trust.

To further explore the relationship of drivers’ attitudes toward imperfect mitigation strategies for different age groups and presentation modalities, a taxonomy was created to systematically identify different mitigation strategies based on different dimensions of automation. This taxonomy, which was initially discussed by Donmez, Boyle, and Lee (2003), embodies a conceptual model and helped guide this research in several ways. First, it provided a classification scheme based on dimensions previously identified as relevant to distraction mitigation; those dimensions are automation level, initiation type, and task relevancy. Second, the classification helped identify gaps in research and areas where additional strategies were needed. Focus groups were then conducted to assess drivers’ acceptance of and trust in all the strategies (existing and innovative) as defined in the taxonomy. A driving simulator study was designed to further assess trust and acceptance using two of the more innovative strategies defined there.

The objective of this study is to understand how imperfect distraction mitigation strategies can influence drivers’ attitudes toward these strategies. It is hypothesized that differences in trust and acceptance will exist among drivers of different age groups, and that these differences will be influenced by presentation modality and system imperfections. Depending on drivers’ perceptions of system benefits, the level of automation may have varying impacts. Older drivers with degraded driving abilities may be more trusting and accepting of high levels of automation compared to younger drivers. Similarly, system imperfections may result in a larger decrement in trust and acceptance for high levels of automation than for low levels.

Section snippets

Categories of mitigation strategies

A taxonomy of driver distraction mitigation strategies was developed based on three dimensions: the level of automation, initiation type, and the task being modulated by the strategy. These dimensions were considered critical for the development of mitigation strategies because of their potential influence on drivers’ responses and attitudes. Twelve unique mitigation strategies were defined and categorized in terms of these dimensions. Based on recent definitions of automation levels (

Assessing acceptance and trust

The focus groups helped us define some key characteristics for mitigation strategies and to determine what types of systems may be more acceptable to drivers. Of the mitigation strategies presented in the taxonomy, the majority of previous research has focused on driving-related strategies. Of the non-driving-related strategies, only demand minimizing has been investigated as a potential means of reducing distraction (Lee, Caven, Haake, & Brown, 2001). Because the number of non-driving-related

General discussion

Focus group and driving simulator studies were conducted to investigate whether adaptive in-vehicle systems designed to mitigate distraction will be accepted by drivers and could thereby reduce the number of crashes and fatalities that occur each year. Because many focus group participants indicated that they have been distracted while driving and did not want to give up their in-vehicle devices unless required to by law, investigating strategies that help reduce the distraction from these

Acknowledgements

This research was conducted as part of the SAVE-IT program under contract by Delphi Corporation and sponsored by the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration—Office of Vehicle Safety Research. The authors acknowledge the technical support and comments provided by Mike Perel of NHTSA and Mary Stearns of the Volpe Center. Additional assistance in the review and preparation of the manuscript was also provided by two anonymous reviewers and individuals at the University of Iowa from the Human

References (41)

  • Donmez, B., Boyle, L., & Lee, J. D. (in press). The impact of distraction mitigation strategies on driving performance....
  • M.R. Endsley

    Measurement of situation awareness in dynamic systems

    Human Factors

    (1995)
  • J.E. Fox et al.

    Effects of age and congestion information accuracy of advanced traveler information systems on user trust and compliance

    Transportation Research Record

    (1998)
  • B.H. Kantowitz et al.

    Driver acceptance of unreliable traffic information in familiar and unfamiliar settings

    Human Factors

    (1997)
  • J. Kray et al.

    Adult age differences in task switching

    Psychology and Aging

    (2000)
  • J.D. Lee et al.

    Speech-based interaction with in-vehicle computers: The effect of speech-based e-mail on drivers’ attention to the road

    Human Factors

    (2001)
  • J.D. Lee et al.

    Collision warning timing, driver distraction, and driver response to imminent rear end collision in a high fidelity driving simulator

    Human Factors

    (2002)
  • J.D. Lee et al.

    Trust in technology: Designing for appropriate reliance

    Human Factors

    (2004)
  • Lerner, N. (2005). Deciding to be distracted. Paper presented at the Driving Assessment Conference,...
  • N. Moray et al.

    Attention and complacency

    Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics

    (2003)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text